Chris Goldspink (2000)
Modelling social systems as complex: Towards a social simulation meta-model
Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation
vol. 3, no. 2,
<https://www.jasss.org/3/2/1.html>
To cite articles published in the Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, please reference the above information and include paragraph numbers if necessary
Received: 3-Feb-00 Accepted: 5-Mar-00 Published: 31-Mar-00
A real or abstract entity that is able to act on itself and its environment; which has a partial representation of its environment; which can, in a multi-agent universe, communicate with other agents; and who's behaviour is a result of its observations, its knowledge and its interactions with the other agents. (Ferber 1989, p. 249)
A natural or artificial entity with sufficient behavioural plasticity to persist in its medium by responding to recurrent perturbation within that medium so as to maintain its organisation[2].
The brain is thus a highly co-operative system: the dense interconnections amongst its components entail that eventually everything going on will be a function of what all the other components are doing (Varela, Thompson & Rosch 1992, p. 94).
an important and pervasive shift is beginning to take place in cognitive science under the very influence of its own research. This shift requires that we move away from the idea of the world as independent and extrinsic to the idea of a world as inseparable from the structure of [mental] processes of self modification. This change in stance does not express a mere philosophical preference; it reflects the necessity of understanding cognitive systems not on the basis of their input and output relationships but by their operational closure (1992, p. 139).
Such systems do not operate by representation. Instead of representing an independent world, they enact a world as a domain of distinctions that is inseparable from the structure embodied by the cognitive system (1992, p. 140).
language is a biological phenomena because it results from the operations of human beings as living systems, but it takes place in the domain of the co-ordinations of actions of the participants, and not in their physiology or neurophysiology...language as a special kind of operation of in co-ordination of actions requires the neurophysiology of the participants but it is not a neurophysiological phenomenon (1988, p. 45).
An autopoietic system capable of interacting with its own states (as an organism with a nervous system) can do, and capable of developing with others a linguistic consensual domain, can treat its own linguistic states as a source of deformations and thus interact linguistically in a closed linguistic domain (1980, p. 121).
![]() |
Figure 1: Venn diagram of agent classes and their relationship |
Note that the possibility of non-biological cognitive agents is a consequence of the way in which cognition is defined and used here while the possibility of non-biological linguistic agents is speculative.
![]() |
Figure 2:The structure of the meta-model medium |
![]() |
Figure 3. Structure of systems of agents |
whenever we engage in social interactions that we label as dialogue or conversation, these constitute autonomous aggregates, which exhibit all the properties of other autonomous units (Varela 1979, p. 269).Thus in human societies, domains of interaction are primarily brought forth and maintained in language.
![]() |
Figure 4. Structure of systems of systems of agents |
The inner feedback of a social system is very often a conservative factor...In internally differentiated societies, social change seems to originate mostly from the interaction of social systems. Social systems always interact through the interactions of their components, i.e. the individuals that constitute the systems (Hejl 1984, p. 76).
![]() |
Figure 5. Systems of structurally coupled agents give rise to domains of interaction |
Note that to say a system is operationally closed is not the same as the concept of a closed system. A closed system is by definition a system that has no input or output of any kind. An operationally closed system may be, and frequently is, systemically 'open' in that it takes in energy, or some other form of input from the environment and produces output, if only in the form of waste products such as heat. The important point is that operationally closed systems are closed 'informationally'-that is, they do not exchange 'information' with the environment. Their behaviour is internally determined and self referenced. The behaviour of such systems is determined by their structure-by the specific properties, configuration and dynamics of the components that comprise them. The response of the system to perturbations will be determined by this structure.
Operationally closed systems, or unities, as Maturana and Varela (1980) have demonstrated, may become 'structurally coupled' to one another and their environment through mutual recurrent perturbation. If these interactions are complementary, that is, maintain the viability of the interacting systems, this mutual adaptation reflects what we might call co-evolution. The strength of the resulting coupling will depend on the internal structures of each unity, in particular their plasticity and sensitivity to certain types of perturbation. Operationally closed systems may influence one another in one or many dimensions and the nature of the response, again depending on the structure, may be discrete, or continuous. Note that there is no determinism between operationally closed systems and the response of one system to another will be determined by its structure only-this will commonly be non-linear. From this the origin of innovation is explicable (Teubner & Willke 1997), as despite becoming coupled, the presence of non-linearity will lead to discontinuous behavioural responses with the potential to trigger reciprocal interaction leading to co-evolution in otherwise inexplicable directions.
2The concept of organisation used here is consistent with that of Maturana and Varela.
3This is a controversial subject (see Mingers 1995, Teubner & Willke 1997, Goldspink 1999)
AXELROD R. 1997, 'Advancing the Art of Simulation in the Social Sciences', in Conte R., Hegselmann R. & Terna P. (eds) Simulating Social Phenomena, Springer, Berlin. p.p. 21-40.
BRASSEL K. H. Möhting M. Schumacher E. & Troitzsh K. G. 1997, 'Can agents Cover All the World'. In Conte R. Hegselmann R. & Terna P. eds. Simulating Social Phenomena, Springer, Berlin, p.p. 55-72
BROOKS R.A. 1991a, 'Intelligence without representation', Artificial Intelligence, No 47, p.p. 139-159.
BROOKS R.A. 1991b, 'Intelligence without reason', Proceedings of the Twelfth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Darling Harbour, Sydney, Australia 24-30 August, Morgan Kaufmann, p.p. 569-595
BURA S., Guerin-Pace F., Mathian H., Pumain D., & Sanders L., 1995 'Cities can be agents to: a model for the evolution of settlement systems', in Gilbert N., & Conte R (eds), Artificial Societies: The Computer Simulation of social life, UCL Press, London. p.p. 86-102
BURRELL G. & Morgan G. 1994, Sociological Paradigms and Organisational Analysis, Virago, London.
CASTELFRANCHI C. 1998 'Through The Minds of the Agents' Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, Vol 1. No. 1, https://www.jasss.org/1/1/5.html
CLANCEY W.J., Smoliar S.W. & Stefik M.J. (eds) 1994, Contemplating Minds, MIT Press, Cambridge MA.
CONTE R., Hegselmann R. & Terna P. (eds.) 1997, Simulating Social Phenomena, Springer, Berlin
CONTE R. & Gilbert N. 1995, 'Computer Simulation for Social Theory', in Gilbert & Conte (eds) Artificial Societies, UCL Press, London, p.p. 1-15
EPSTEIN J.M. & Axtell R. 1996, Growing Artificial Societies, MIT Press, Cam. Ma. EVE R.A., Horsfall S & Lee M.E., 1997, Chaos, Complexity and Sociology: Myths Models and Theories, Sage
FERBER J. 1989, Des objets aux agents. Doctoral thesis, University of Paris VI.
GILBERT N. & Conte R. eds. 1995, Artificial Societies: The Computer Simulation of Social Life, UCL Press, London.
GILBERT N. & Troitzsch K. G. 1999, Simulation for the Social Scientist, Open University Press, Buckingham.
GOLDSPINK C. 1999, Social Attractors: Applicability of Complexity theory to social and organisational analysis, Unpublished thesis, University of Western Sydney - Hawkesbury, New South Wales, Australia.
GOLDSPINKC 2000, 'Contrasting linear and non-linear perspectives in contemporary social research: Organisation Theory', Submitted for consideration to Emergence.
HEJL P.M. 1984 'Towards a Theory of Social Systems: Self-Organization, Self-Maintenance, Self-Reference and Syn-Reference', in Ulrich H. & Probst G.J.B. eds, Self-Organisation and Management of Social Systems: Insights, Promises, Doubts and Questions, Springer-Verlag, Berlin.
HODGSON G. M. 1996, Economics and Institutions, Polity Press, Oxford
HUTCHINS E. & Hazlehurst B 1995, 'How to invent a lexicon: the development of shared symbols', in Interaction, in Artificial Societies: The Computer Simulation of Social Life, Gilbert N. & Conte R. (eds.), UCL Press, London, p.p. 157-189.
KAUFFMAN S. A.1993 The Origins of Order: Self Organization and Selection in Evolution, Oxford University Press
KAUFFMAN S.1996, At home in the Universe: The Search for Laws of Complexity, Penguin, London
KAUFFMAN S.& Macready W. 1995, 'Technological Evolution and Adaptive Organizations', Complexity, Vol 1 No. 2., p.p. 26-43.
MACY M. W. 1998, 'Social Order in Artificial Worlds', Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, Vol. 1, No. 1, http:jasss.soc.surrey.ac.uk/1/1/4.html
MARION R. 1999, The Edge of Organisation: Chaos and Complexity Theories of Formal Social Systems, Sage.
MCKELVEY 1997, 'Quasi-Natural Organisation Science', Organization Science, No 8, p.p. 351-380
MCKELVEY1999, 'Complexity Theory in Organization Science: Seizing the Promise or Becoming a Fad?', Emergence, Vol 1 No 1., p.p. 5-32
MATURANA H.R. 1987, 'Everything is Said By an Observer', In Thompson W.I. (ed.) 1987, Gaia: A Way of Knowing, Lindisfarne Press, Barrington, MA, p.p. 65-82
MATURANA H.R.1988, 'Reality: The Search for Objectivity of the Quest for Compelling Argument', The Irish Journal of Psychology, Vol. 9 No. 1. p.p. 25-82
MATURANA H.R.& Varela F. 1980, Autopoiesis and Cognition, Reidel
MATURANA H.R.& Varela F. 1988, The Tree of Knowledge - The Biological Roots of Human Understanding, Shambhala
MATURANA H.R.Mpodozis J & Letelier J.C. 1995, 'Brain, Language and the Origin of Human Mental Functions', Biological Research, Vol.. 28, p.p. 15-26,
MINGERS, J. 1995, Self-producing Systems: Implications and Applications of Autopoiesis, Plenum Press, New York.
MINSKY M. 1987, The Society of Mind, Picador, London.
ORMEROD P. 1995, The Death of Economics, Faber and Faber, London
ORMEROD P. 1998, Butterfly Economics, Faber & Faber, London
PLOTKIN H. 1994, The Nature of Knowledge, Allen Lane
PORTUGALI J., Benenson I & Omer I, 1997, 'Spatial Cognitive Dissonance and Sociospacial Emergence in a Self-organizing City', Environment and Planning, Vol 24, p.p. 263-285
ROOS J. & von Krogh G. 1995, Organizational Epistemology, St. Martins Press, N.Y.
SOMMERHOFF G. 1974[1969], 'The Abstract Characteristics of Living Systems', in Emery F.E. (ed.) Systems Thinking, Penguin. p.p. 147-202
TERNA P. 1998, 'Simulation Tools for Social Scientists: Building Agent Based Models with SWARM', Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, Vol. 1, No. 2, https://www.jasss.org/1/2/4.html
TEUBNER G & Willke H. 1997, 'Can Social Systems be Viewed as Autopoietic?', LSE Study Group, Report on Presentations, Meeting No. 3, 18 June.
TROITZSCH K.G. 1997, 'Social Science Simulation-Origins, Prospects, Purposes', in Conte R., Hegselmann R. & Terna P. (eds) Simulating Social Phenomena, Springer, Berlin. p.p 41-54
VARELA F. 1979, Principles of Biological Autonomy, North Holland, Amsterdam.
Varela F. (1981) 'Describing the logic of the Living. The adequacy and limitations of the idea of Autopoiesis' in Autopoiesis: A theory of the Living Organization M. Zeleny (de) Elsevier-Verlag, Berlin.
VARELA F. 1984, 'Two principles of self-organization', in Ulrich H. & Probst G.J.B. eds, Self-Organisation and Management of Social Systems Insights, Promises, Doubts and Questions, Springer-Verlag, Berlin., p.p. 25-32
VARELA F., Thompson E. & Rosch E. 1992, The Embodied Mind, MIT Press, Ca. Mass.
WATT S., 1996, Artificial Societies and Psychological Agents, Knowledge Media Institute, The Open University
WILSON E.O. 1975, Sociobiology: The new synthesis, Belknap Press, CA
WINOGRAD T. & Flores F. 1986, Understanding Computers and Cognition: A New Foundation for Design, Ablex, Norwood, NJ.
WHITAKER R. (1996), 'Autopoietic Theory and Social Systems: Theory and Practice', http://www.acm.org/sigois/auto/AT&Soc.html#Luhmann
WOOLDRIDGE, M. J., and Jennings, N. R. 1995. 'Intelligent agents: Theory and practice'. Knowledge Engineering Review, Vol 10 No 2. p.p. 49-62
Return to Contents of this issue
© Copyright Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, 1999